(psychology, self help) A fuller explanation of cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT)


A fuller explanation of CBT
John Winston Bush, PhD
New York Institute for Cognitive and Behavioral Therapies
The cognitive side of CBT
Perhaps this will help make it clear. You must have noticed that when you are experiencing an
emotion, your body feels different. This is because you re sensing certain distinctive changes in
your internal physiology. It s no accident that the word  feeling can be a synonym for
 emotion. In other words (to simplify things a bit) . . .
To have an emotion is to feel the physical (bodily)
consequences of our thoughts.
Imagine the following situation:
A friend is due to meet you for dinner at your house at 7:00. But it s now past 8:00, and there s been
no sign of her  not even a phone call. How are you going to feel about this?
Well, as this diagram makes clear, there s more than one possible answer:
What you think How you feel What you do
"She might have been Worried or anxious Call hospital ERs to find
hurt on the way here." out if she s there
"She didn t bother to let Annoyed or angry Chew her out, or act
me know she was chilly, when she does
Friend is late for
delayed." show up
dinner
"It doesn t matter to me Indifferent Nothing in particular
whether people are on
time."
"I needed the time to fix Relieved Relax and enjoy yourself
the house up anyway."
Now of course there are ways not shown in the diagram in which someone might interpret a friend s
being late, and different way  as a result  in which he or she might react emotionally and
behaviorally.
Note also that your thoughts about your friend s lateness don t affect just your feelings  they can
also influence the actions you take.
And while it might seem silly to consult a psychotherapist over nothing more than a dinner date, the
basic principle is exactly the same when it comes to major and more complex problems.
As the philosopher Epictetus said almost 2,000 years ago:
"The thing that upsets people is not what happens
but what they think it means."
People  and I mean all people, not just therapy clients  routinely distress themselves and others
with arbitrary interpretations of what is going on. Sometimes this is done out of blind habit, or under
the influence of a bad mood or bodily discomfort; sometimes it happens for quite other reasons.
Challenging, and at times changing, one s doubtful interpretations of events is much of the cognitive
work of CBT.
It s important not to get this confused with the ever-popular practice of  positive thinking. In CBT
the goal is accurate and rational thinking  the kind that is based as much as possible on logic and
the available facts. Most of the time this does result in a more positive outlook  but there are also
times when its value lies in correcting an undesirably rosy view of things.
On the other hand . . .
So far, we ve been looking at how our thinking influences our feelings and behavior. Before going
on, however, we also need to look at the converse  how our feelings and behavior influence our
thinking.
For a wide-ranging review of the first part of this vital topic, see Stanford psychologist
Gordon Bower s speech and monograph, Emotion and Social Judgments. (File size is
55KB, but well worth it  there s enough food for thought in this paper to keep you
topped up for weeks.)
If something happens to which you automatically and reflexively react with fear or anxiety, your
thoughts will tend to be about danger and the consequences of being harmed. Similarly, if your
instant reaction to an event is to feel angry or sad or happy  and especially if you also act on your
feelings  your thoughts (including your recollections of the past and your vision of the future) will
be biased in the same direction
Emotional reactions arise chiefly in a region of the brain called the limbic system, which is very
fast-acting and can respond to events on the basis of quick-and-dirty impressions. This nimbleness
of response has survival value in some situations  such as noticing a fast-moving object that
could be a car or truck approaching as you cross a street.
But on other occasions  such as complex situations where you need to call on more of your
accumulated knowledge and experience, speed can be a disadvantage. The more complete
information you need at such times has to be processed by the lateral prefrontal cortex of your brain
 which gets into gear about half a second later than your limbic system. This short delay is often
enough that your thinking, under the influence of an automated emotional reaction, heads off in a
biased direction that you may come to regret.
In instances like these, the influence of thinking on emotions can be one of sustaining or amplifying
an emotion as opposed to initiating it. (To see how this works, remember some occasion when
something made you hopping mad, and you then fed the feeling with minutes or hours of angry
thoughts  only to discover later on that there had simply been a misunderstanding.)
In practice, treating this kind of problem clinically involves methods traditionally associated, not with
cognitive therapy, but with behavior therapy. To which we now turn.
The behavioral side of CBT
You ve probably heard about the Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov. The one who taught dogs to
salivate when they heard a buzzer. Since we re going to be talking about Pavlov s contributions to
psychotherapy, you may as well know that he looked exactly like this guy with the cool Edwardian
beard . . . .
Much, though far from all, of behavior therapy derives from Pavlov s
demonstration that events occurring closely together in time are likely to be
stored in the brain in a sort of mental package. Because Pavlov set off the
buzzer just as he was about to give the dog some food, the buzzer and the food
became associated with each other. As a result, after a while the dog began
salivating when he heard a buzzer  whether he was given food or not.
The next thing Pavlov discovered was that if he sounded the buzzer too often
without coming through with some food, the dog no longer salivated just
because there was a buzzer buzzing. In behavior jargon, this is called
 extinction. It refers to the fact that a conditioned reaction  in humans as
well as dogs  can become substantially overridden if it is no longer "reinforced."
(Reinforcement, in the Pavlovian learning model, means that some event like the sounding of a
buzzer  which doesn t naturally bring forth a reaction such as a salivating  is experienced at the
same time as something that does, such as the sight or smell of food. When this happens, the event
can become an artificial cue or signal that triggers something resembling the natural response.)
P.S.: Recent developments in learning theory  corroborated by recordings of
dopamine-connected neurons of the brain s  reward system  suggest that Pavlov s dogs
didn t learn to salivate just because they heard the buzzer at the same time as they received food.
They learned it when the arrival of food came as something of a surprise, since they hadn t
previously expected buzzers to be a signal for food. Makes sense: if they d already known about the
connection, what would there have been for them to learn?
A couple of practical examples
For example, if you are deathly afraid of riding in elevators, we can usually extinguish that fear if you
are willing to crank up your courage and take one elevator ride after another* until you are no longer
unreasonably afraid. It works because your conditioned fear reaction is not being reinforced  that
is, the elevator doesn t fall or get stuck for hours.
(On the other hand, if you give in to the fear and avoid elevators, you can pretty much count on
spending the rest of your life being afraid of them.)
Or, to give another example, let s imagine that you become depressed following a setback such as
the loss of a loved one or the collapse of your efforts to achieve some valued goal.
You may feel that it s useless to try to live a normal life, since your energy and ability to enjoy things
seem to have vanished. You probably expect that pursuing your goals will merely lead to
disappointment, frustration or failure  in other words, you have a sense of futility. Acting on these
feelings, you drop out of your usual activities and social relations. The result: your life becomes
even more constricted and unrewarding, and your morale goes still deeper into the hole.
Getting back to normal
What we are likely to do in CBT is move you gradually back towards leading a normal life, without
waiting for it to feel as good as it once did. If you diligently follow this plan, the renewed contact with
your friends, family and regular activities should eventually bring your mood and feelings back to
normal. This is because before you became depressed, your usual activities and relationships were
associated with more energy and enjoyment than you are feeling now. We are trying to take
advantage of those connections as a means of bootstrapping your morale to a more satisfactory
level.
(You might like to see the report of a 1996 study of treatment for depression
suggesting that this kind of behavioral intervention may be as effective as full-scale
cognitive behavior therapy for depression.)
Other behavioral aspects of CBT have nothing to do with Pavlov or "conditioning" of any kind. For
instance, we often work with patients to experiment with taking some action that is likely to prove
beneficial and instructive. Or, right in the office, to enact (rather than just talk about) an
interpersonal or internal conflict, in the manner of Gestalt therapy "chair work" or psychodrama.
These have been just a few illustrations of the hundreds of behavioral interventions that are
possible. But perhaps now you have at least some feel for the  B in CBT.
Why cognitive? Why behavioral?
Everyone, including cognitive behavior therapists, understands that emotions and moods are
governed by factors in addition to one s own thinking and behavior. However, as a practical matter
virtually the only means of access to our moods and emotions are the cognitive and behavioral
routes.
In order to voluntarily change how we feel, we have to go about it indirectly, not directly. There is no
direct way to influence our feelings and moods. Brains simply aren t built so as to make this
possible.
Now, according to Prof. James Gross of Stanford, a leading researcher in the field of emotion
regulation, there are five points in the generation of an emotion at which it may be possible to
exercise deliberate influence:
selection of the situation
modification of the situation
deployment of attention
change of cognitions
modulation of responses
The first, second and fifth of these points are targeted in behavior therapy, the third and fourth in
cognitive therapy. As you no doubt suspect, most of the time some combination of behavioral and
cognitive methods is needed.
As an experiment, just try to change whatever mood or emotional state you are in right now . . . .
OK, did you succeed? Then how did you do it? Was it mainly cognitive, mainly behavioral, both 
or neither one?
Mainly Mainly
What you did
Cognitive Behavioral
You thought or imagined something that would support the mood
or feeling you wanted
You called up a memory of a time when you felt that way
You redirected your attention to other thoughts, images or
activities
You performed some physical action, such as walking around the
room or working out
You meditated, or did a deep-breathing or relaxation exercise
You talked to someone, perhaps trying to get them to do
something that would change how you felt
You hypnotized yourself, or got some else to hypnotize you
You took a drink, smoked a cigarette, played some music, or
popped a Valium
No, what I did was...
? ?
If you did something that you believe was a direct, not an indirect way to change your feeling state,
please send an e-mail to me using this link. I d like to know what you came up with.
Finally, a word about one of the behavioral methods listed above  trying to get other people to
change their thinking and behavior:
Inducing other people to change their minds, or to act differently, can do wonders for how one feels.
However, in practice it is often easier  and can be more satisfying in the long run  to alter one s
own thinking and behavior. And in any case the main obstacle to influencing others to behave as we
would like can easily be our own thinking and behavior.
For here is the central secret of behavior modification:
In order to modify somebody else s behavior,
you first have to modify your own.
In other words, if you keep on doing the same thing, you ll most likely get the same result. Same old
 > same old  > same old.
Hey! Whatever happened to . . .
. . . Dreams?
Some cognitive behavior therapists (including myself) do in fact work with patients dream
recollections. After all, research has demonstrated that cognition of a sort can go on outside of
awareness.
But the  cognitive unconscious, as it is called in experimental psychology, differs radically from
the Freudian version. And Freud s claim to have uncovered the secrets of dreaming was sheer
chutzpah  there were so many interpretive wildcards in his published case histories that it is a
wonder they were ever taken seriously.
Dreams sometimes do point to thoughts  and the emotions associated with them  in ways that
are therapeutically useful, and might go unnoticed otherwise. And sometimes they don t. Their value
is real but in general rather limited, and certainly doesn t justify the central role they are given in
psychoanalysis and its derivatives.
. . . My horrible childhood?
If you have good reason to think your childhood was horrible, it may indeed have had something to
do with your need for therapy now. But people can develop psychiatric symptoms for other reasons,
and not all of them are readily explainable. Moreover, there are people whose childhoods were
perfectly appalling  yet they weren t severely affected. (This can happen if they were lucky
enough to have been born with genes for a hardy nervous system  or if, despite having a
messed-up family, they got involved with sane and supportive peers.)
The point is that every problem must have begun somewhere, and discussing how it began may or
may not be helpful in helping to overcome it. Some patients profit from a certain amount of
reminiscing and emoting about childhood events, while for others it is largely a waste of time.
Quite often, in fact, thinking and talking about past troubles turns out to be downright harmful. It can
lead to emotional pain that serves no useful purpose. It can lower your stress threshold so that you
make mistakes that could have been avoided. And it can divert your attention to past events about
which nothing can any longer be done  at the expense of your present situation and the
opportunities it offers for constructive change.
In CBT, if childhood events are given much attention, the point is not usually to explain today s
predicaments in terms of the past. This exercise has proven to be of limited value therapeutically.
The chief value (when there is any) in going over disturbing memories is to identify repeating
patterns that can give the therapy more focus and make it more efficient.
A second goal, for some people, would be to provide  exposure without reinforcement  i.e.,
habituation of painful emotions that serve no good purpose and are undermining your efforts to lead
a good life. As part of this process, troubling events that you have not yet talked freely and
reflectively about ( unfinished business ) can be put into perspective, so that they no longer act
as a drain on your mental and physical resources.
A further difficulty with memories of childhood is that they aren t necessarily accurate, let alone
unbiased. Memory is a tricky thing, particularly when strong emotions are involved. Therapy needs
as much as possible to be grounded in the truth, and too much reliance on memories of the past 
especially the remote past  can result in countertherapeutic distortions.
. . . Id, ego and superego?
Actually, if you go back and read the German, you will discover that Freud s words were das Es,
das Ich and das Überich
. English being a Germanic language, these terms would better be
translated as the It, the Me and the Over-me. But James Strachey, Freud s first English translator,
thought he had to drag Latin into it, even though his native German was good enough for Freud
himself.
Goes to show what happens when you let one of that Bloomsbury crowd have the assignment.
However you translate the terms, they stand for extremely broad categories of mind and behavior.
So broad, in fact, that they are often used in ways that seem profound but don t really mean
anything. Which reminds me of a stock joke in psychology that goes like this:
Q. What is a psychoanalytic diagnosis?
A. It is a way of describing the patient s problems such that absolutely nothing can be done about
them. And to do this will take several years at three times a week.
. . . The couch?
There is a couch (brown) in my office. Its uses are, in order of importance . . .
1st For people to sit on.
2nd To fill that otherwise empty-looking spot in the room.
3rd For people to lie on while I teach them "progressive relaxation."
4th For doing occasional hypnotherapy.
5th For a few minutes  not a whole hour  of free association or something
(and last) resembling it.
. . . The inferiority complex?
Most people have at least a touch of it. (This was Alfred Adler s idea, not Freud s.)
And most of them don t. (Freud was vain enough not to have one, so he and Adler fought about it.)
. . . The obligatory picture of Freud on the wall?
By now you have doubtless decided that I don t have one of those famous totems in the office.
You re right  I don t. But when I m seeing a new patient who has spent years in psychoanalysis
and (like Woody Allen) had almost nothing to show for it, there is now a picture that does my
indignation justice.
The picture is of Freud s statue being installed on the campus of
Clark University in Worcester, Massachusetts, where Freud
gave his only American lectures. (Several members of the
Psychology faculty objected to the monument  but they lost
the argument to the university s president, who thought it would
lend prestige to the institution, i.e., help him raise money.)
To give the man fair credit, it has to be said that he was the first
person to make psychotherapy a popular subject. More
important, though, he and his daughter Anna were the first to
give a systematic account of  ego defenses  the various
stunts we use to preserve our reputations, both publicly and in
the privacy of our own minds, for being ethical and competent
people.
As social animals, we are faced with strong incentives to justify
our actions to other people  and as language-using animals,
we have a unique and efficient means of doing so. This,
however, creates various kinds of problems when our
justifications are untrue, or at least are less than the whole story.
These problems in turn provide a good deal of employment for
therapists, whether they are of the psychodynamic persuasion or the behavioral.
On the minus side, Freud s celebrated case studies have turned out to be largely inventions of his
own imagination. He was not only vain, but a master of sophistry, self-deception and salesmanship.
He indulged in unfettered and highly subjective speculation  and proclaimed his conjectures to be
laws of human nature. And he developed a cult-like following that persists to this day. One of his
severest critics, Nobel-winning scientist Sir Peter Medawar, put it like this:  Psychoanalysis is the
most stupendous intellectual confidence trick of the 20th century.
In short, while Freud s work advanced clinical psychology and psychiatry in some respects, in other
ways it obstructed their development by several decades.
For a more complete account of how Freud s theorizing led the mental health field
astray, follow this link.
(On a personal note, I was trained in psychodynamic therapy and practiced it for years, but grew
increasingly dissatisfied with its effectiveness. In my struggle to find something better, I managed to
invent a primitive form of CBT. Then I discovered, by reading research reports I d previously
overlooked, that such an ambitious project wouldn t be necessary: that particular wheel had already
been invented. There went the Nobel I was dreaming of, right up the spout. But life does go on,
doesn t it? Sigh.)
For a comprehensive critique of Freud and psychoanalysis, you might want to read
Frederick Crews s book, Unauthorized Freud: Doubters Confront a Legend. There are
many other books in this genre, but for most people Crews s is the best place to start.
You can order a copy from Amazon.com by clicking on the  Books I often
recommend link in the Table of Contents. Or, come to think of it, you can order it
right from here. But hurry back  because as you will see below, there s more to
come that s worth your reading and thinking about.
And if you d like to see psychoanalysis and CBT compared point-by-point, click here
to see an interesting excerpt from Davison and Neale s leading textbook, Abnormal
Psychology, 8th edition.
Now don t go away  there s more, and it s better still. The next page is Learning theory: A
fuller-fuller explanation of CBT. As long as you ve come this far, why not find out still more? There
are good reasons why CBT works so well, and this page will tell you about some of the main ones.
Copyright © 1996-2003 John Winston Bush, PhD. All rights reserved.
Page updated 2004 Jan 03.
Contacts: 718 636-5071 and jwb@alumni.stanford.org.


Wyszukiwarka

Podobne podstrony:
Folk Psychology and the Explanation of Human Behavior
(Psychology, Self Help) How Others Influence Our Self Esteem
(psychology, self help) Myths and Facts about Depression and Bipolar Disorder
(psychology, self help) Intimacy in Relationships
(psychology, self help) Shyness and Social Anxiety A Self Help Guide
Cognitive behavior therapy for mood disorders
Investor Psychology A Behavioral Explanation Of Six Finance Puzzles
Chomsky, Noam Philosophy Of Cognitive Science
(ebook self help pdf) Hypnosis The Subconscious Mind
(Psychology, Self
3 Psychologia self i teoria relacji z obiektem
Self ignition and ignition of aluminum powders in shock waves
Cognitive and behavioral therapies
eBook Self Help Verbal Advantage Level 1
Simulation of the Behavior of Mixtures of Heavy Particles Behind a Shock Wave Front

więcej podobnych podstron